In the current animal model of depression, a mouse is placed
in a jar of water and struggles to swim to avoid drowning, in the aptly named “forced-swim”
test(1). After a few minutes, the
mouse stops trying to escape, instead choosing to float immobile in the water.
At this point, the mouse is said to experience “behavioural despair” (the mouse
loses hope to escape the stressful environment) and the mouse is then
classified as suffering from “depression”. This is the standard model used to
test antidepressant drugs – the time spent immobile versus swimming in mice
given the drug is compared to that of controls. Clearly, this is a simplistic model with very little resemblance to the highly complex,
multi-faceted disorder of clinical depression in humans. Yet, although its
efficacy has long been contested(2), this is still the most
common mouse model used in the majority of research into “depression”.
This is not just the case for depression. Many psychiatric
disorders are still studied using simplistic animal models which – though important
– essentially bear little resemblance to the experiences of those suffering
from the disease on a daily basis.
On top of this there, is a pervasive disconnect between
psychology and neuroscience. It is fundamentally impossible to study affective
or cognitive phenomena such as emotion or foresight using animal models, since
mice, rats and monkeys are all unable to communicate what they are feeling with
humans. Instead, researchers study behaviours
as a proxy – for instance, what a mouse does
before it gets a reward, which is largely a matter of interpretation. But we
lack a way to study actual emotions in mice – which, besides, are likely vastly
different subjective experiences to those of humans. While mouse models have
their uses, they are generally an insufficient representation of brain disease
or even normal brain function in humans.
It is without surprise then, that over the past 40 years
there has been little improvements in the outcomes of patients with the most
common brain diseases. Some pharmaceutical companies are abandoning research
into drugs for psychiatric diseases altogether due to the high cost and low
success rate. For example for Alzheimer’s, every time we think we have a
promising new drug in development to break down the toxic amyloid plaques, we
find that it fails in clinical trials, and moreover, we find that we were
coming at the problem from the wrong angle altogether(3). We now know that we need to
intervene long before amyloid deposits are prevalent, and long before symptoms
are seen. Some research shows a portion of patients diagnosed with Alzheimer’s
do not even have significantly more amyloid-β plaques in their brains than healthy controls, and amyloid pathology has been observed in cognitively healthy
elderly individuals, suggesting that amyloid-driven tauopathy may at best only
be part of the problem. Thus, we currently lack even an effective diagnostic
criteria for neurodegenerative disorders. Treatment for Alzheimer’s is largely
symptomatic; we are a long way off from understanding the root causes of the
disease. Progress is slow, but we are learning.
There is also the problem of brain scanning. The most
prevalent form of brain imaging in neuroscience and psychology is undoubtedly
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). However, again, this is a proxy –
fMRI measures blood flow across the brain while the subject is engaged in a
particular task or activity; it does not directly measure neuronal activity(4). One unpublished study from
2009 found apparent cognitive activity in the brain of a dead salmon(5), highlighting the risk of
false positives in fMRI studies. Similarly, electroencephalography (EEG)
measures electrical activity at the brain surface – however it lacks
specificity in that it does not measure the activity of specific neurons or
sets of neurons, but rather of a crude combination of electrical currents
across a particular brain area. Unfortunately, it is not yet possible to
measure the activity of a specific set of neurons in living, human brain
tissue.
However, a small minority of forward-thinking neuroengineers
are currently working on measuring real-time electrical brain activity in vivo, in humans. This is already
possible in the brains of mice and in monkeys, but not yet in humans. Thus,
hopefully in the not-too-distant future, we will be able to record activity
from specific sub-sets of neurons and correlate this with not only behaviour,
but with thought, emotion and, of course, depression.
With a little imagination, let’s fast forward 100, or
perhaps only 50 years. We now understand the root causes of Alzheimer’s,
Parkinson’s, depression, schizophrenia etc., and are able to deliver targeted
genetic or drug therapies, custom-made for each patient, to treat brain disease
both symptomatically, and more importantly, prophylactically. Furthermore, we
now understand that these disorders which we considered one disease, were in
fact different diseases with similar symptoms but vastly different biological
causes, each requiring a different treatment. We will look back to the
primitive days of neuroscience – the early 21st century – and be
amazed that the majority of brain diseases were being treated with the wrong
drugs, which were more often than not completely ineffective, or even counter-productive(6,7).
In order to achieve this, we first had to figure out how to
get electrodes through the skull and into the brains of healthy, living humans,
without causing any risk to the subject. Rather than drilling holes through the
skull, we use microelectrodes so small that they can be inserted without
rupturing any blood vessels, thus avoiding the risk of stroke. We might even
use lasers. As technology progresses, we will be able to record from thousands
of electrodes at once using smart, robotic, microscopic implantations which
work their way around blood vessels and through the brain tissue. Eventually,
this will be possible using wearable devices which the subject can implant into
their brain and go about their day, while the device is constantly collecting
and uploading high-resolution neuronal activity directly from their brain to
the computer of a researcher, or their doctor, for analysis. Combined with
powerful yet harmless lasers able to pass through the skull and produce images
of neurons and synapses with sub-cellular precision, we are able to decipher
not only the connectome, but the precise patterns of activity between specific
neurons during a particular function, on an individual level, for each patient.
By collecting masses of data from millions of patients, we can mathematically
calculate – with the aid of superfast computers – what exactly is going wrong
in neurological diseases such as Alzheimer’s. Furthermore, we have finally
managed to bridge the gap between psychology and neuroscience, by being able to
ask the patients about their emotional, subjective experiences, and correlate this with their neuronal activity at that exact point in time.
At some point, these wearable devices will become
commercialised by the likes of Google, Amazon and Apple, offering free services
to customers in exchange for their private data – their thoughts. Having
learned from our mistakes in the early 21st century regarding
privacy and data harvesting, customers will demand rights and legislation to decide
how their personal brain activity is used by multinational corporations.
However, this will prove ineffective, and customers will willingly sacrifice
their privacy anyway, by updating to the latest version of Apple iBrain®
without reading the terms and conditions. This will open up a whole Pandora’s
box of neuro-hacking and neuro-spyware, as well as further driving inequality
and elitism – since only those in first-world countries can access the devices,
and only the wealthiest of those can afford the latest and greatest
bio-upgrades. The societal, political and economic ramifications of this could
fill an entire book in and of itself. But from a neuroscientific perspective,
this will be a turning-point; a revolution in neuroscience research, allowing for
not only the enhancement of normal brain function – or biohacking – but also significant
advances in the treatment of brain diseases. Alzheimer’s, schizophrenia,
autism, ADHD, addiction, depression and anxiety disorders will all be things of the past.
So too will smartphones. Generation Y will tell their kids,
“I remember when we had to type our text
messages with our thumbs, or ask Alexa to add vegan meat to the shopping list.
We never had Google Think® in my day”, or “I remember when we had to go to college to study for years, we had to
sit down and read books to learn things. We never had Amazon HiveMind® in my
day”. Meanwhile their kids seamlessly communicate via Apple iThought®,
video chat via Skype Hologram®, and instantaneously download entire textbooks
and literature via an ultra-fast 100Gb/s subscription to Amazon’s entire
library for only $19.99 per month. Fake news will become a thing of the past,
as every news article you download is instantly verified against thousands of
peer-reviewed sources – reviewed both by humans and by sophisticated AI
technology. You will never forget anything ever again, as any memory you choose
to remember will be uploaded to the cloud, ready to be accessed and relived at
will. Alternatively, should you choose, you can delete a traumatic or stressful
memory, like it never happened. Without delving too far into the realm of
science fiction, the possibilities are Limitless®. Anything is possible, so
long as we can dream it – or Google DeepDream® it.
Our knowledge of neuroscience is only in its infancy. Our
understanding the human brain in all its complexity is only a mere few steps
away from exponential growth. As technology combines with neuroscience, we
become ever closer to understanding ourselves, and to an entirely interconnected
consciousness. Societies working together as a collective intelligence are
capable of amazing things – just look at bees and ants. Times are changing, for
better or for worse.
Now, back to those mice...
References:
1. Can, Adem, Dao, David T., Arad,
Michal, Terrillion, Chantelle E., et al. (2012) ‘The Mouse Forced Swim Test’. Journal
of Visualized Experiments : JoVE, (59). [online] Available from:
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3353513/
2. Borsini, Franco,
Volterra, Giovanna and Meli, Alberto (1986) ‘Does the behavioral “despair” test
measure “despair”?’ Physiology & Behavior, 38(3), pp. 385–386.
3. Castello, Michael
A., Jeppson, John David and Soriano, Salvador (2014) ‘Moving beyond anti-amyloid
therapy for the prevention and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease’. BMC
Neurology, 14, p. 169.
4. Ekstrom, Arne
(2010) ‘How and when the fMRI BOLD signal relates to underlying neural
activity: The danger in dissociation’. Brain Research Reviews, 62(2),
pp. 233–244.
5. Scicurious (2012)
‘IgNobel Prize in Neuroscience: The dead salmon study’. Scientific American
Blog Network. [online] Available from:
http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/scicurious-brain/ignobel-prize-in-neuroscience-the-dead-salmon-study/
(Accessed 27 April 2016)
6. Anon (2016) ‘Most
antidepressant drugs ineffective for children and teens, study finds’. University
of Oxford. [online] Available from:
http://www.ox.ac.uk/news/2016-06-08-most-antidepressant-drugs-ineffective-children-and-teens-study-finds
(Accessed 6 July 2018)
7. Cipriani, Andrea, Zhou, Xinyu, Giovane, Cinzia Del, Hetrick, Sarah E., et al. (2016) ‘Comparative efficacy and tolerability of antidepressants for major depressive disorder in children and adolescents: a network meta-analysis’. The Lancet, 388(10047), pp. 881–890.
7. Cipriani, Andrea, Zhou, Xinyu, Giovane, Cinzia Del, Hetrick, Sarah E., et al. (2016) ‘Comparative efficacy and tolerability of antidepressants for major depressive disorder in children and adolescents: a network meta-analysis’. The Lancet, 388(10047), pp. 881–890.
No comments:
Post a Comment